Causality Assessment: How do we know that meditation is the cause?

In the Varieties of Contemplative Experience study, causal attribution to meditation was assessed according to the causality assessment criteria that regulatory agencies such as the World Health Organization (WHO), the Federal Drug Administration (FDA), and the National Institutes of Health (NIH) use to make health policy decisions (Agbabiaka, Savovic, & Ernst, 2008; NIH, 2016; Turner, 1984; WHO, 2016). These criteria are designed to assess causality of treatment-related adverse events in individual cases in the absence of prospective or epidemiological (base rate) data. Thirteen standard causality criteria typically include:

7) re-challenge,

8) specificity,

9-11) three types of consistency,

12) biological gradient

13) linkage to known biological mechanisms

1) prior published reports,

2) expert judgment,

3) subjective attribution,

4) temporal proximity (challenge),

5) exacerbation,

6) de-challenge,

(Agbabiaka et al., 2008; Gallagher et al., 2011; Hill, 1965, 2015; Naranjo, 1986; Naranjo, Busto, & Sellers, 1982; Naranjo et al., 1981; Theophile et al., 2010).

The Varieties of Contemplative Experiencet study assessed the first 11 criteria. Prior published reports included case reports or studies about unusual or challenging meditation-related experiences in the scientific or clinical literature. Expert judgment was derived from interviews with 32 meditation teachers and clinicians. The following six criteria were assessed as part of the demographics and attributes follow-up questionnaire or, in the case of non-responders, extracted from the interview transcript:

  1. causal attribution to meditation by the subject (subjective attribution);

  2. temporal proximity to (either during or following) meditation practice (challenge);

  3. exacerbation of pre-existing symptoms following meditation;

  4. occurrence on more than one occasion (consistency);

  5. decrease when practice is reduced (de-challenge);

  6. re-appearance when practice is repeated (re-challenge).

A causality score was calculated as the sum of endorsements of these six criteria. Using standard guidelines (OHRP, 2007), a score of two or greater, signifying “possibly related,” was the cutoff for inclusion.

Specificity—or the likelihood of a cause other than meditation—was evaluated at two levels.  If an alternative cause such as medical illness or pre-existing psychological conditions could have wholly accounted for the experiences reported, then these subjects failed to meet causality criteria and were excluded.  In the remaining practitioners, specific experiences that could not be directly linked to meditation or that were attributed to other causes (e.g., drug use, prior psychiatric or medical history, or a period in life prior to learning meditation) were not coded for phenomenology (see below: Phenomenology coding). 

The consistency criterion was assessed at three levels: intra-subjective, inter-subjective, and cross-modal. Intra-subjective consistency refers to the same or a similar experience occurring in close temporal proximity (during or following) to meditation on more than one occasion within the same individual. Inter-subjective consistency refers to the same or a similar experience occurring during or following meditation in multiple individuals. Cross-modal consistency refers to different classes of sources (practitioners and experts) reporting the same or a similar experience during or following meditation (i.e. source triangulation).

Prior published reports: See Bibliography for up-to-date count of published reports of meditation-related difficulties in the scientific, clinical and medical literature. Current count(as of 9.19.19) = 80.

Expert judgment was derived from interviews with 32 meditation teachers and clinicians who reported 56 categories of meditation-related experiences that they had observed in their students.

Through the causality assessment section of the demographic and attributes follow-up questionnaire, practitioners reported on six causality criteria: subjective attribution, temporal proximity (challenge), exacerbation, consistency, de-challenge, and re-challenge. Meditation practitioners met an average of four causality criteria (mean = 4.2 ±1.1 range = 2-6;), with more than half (60%) meeting four, five, or all six criteria, well-exceeding the minimum cutoff of two that standard guidelines use to warrant further investigation (OHRP, 2007).

The consistency criterion was assessed on three levels: In the current study, 88% of the practitioner sample met the intra-subjective consistency criterion, indicating that they reported having the same or a similar experience during or following meditation on multiple occasions.

Inter-subjective consistency was assessed through evaluating how many practitioners reported each category (see Table 4 above). Each category of experience was reported by an average of 20 practitioners, indicating that the same or a similar experience occurring in temporal proximity to meditation was reported by multiple individuals. Cross-modal consistency was assessed by comparing the phenomenology reported in practitioner interviews to the phenomenology reported in expert interviews. Fifty-six of 59 (95%) categories of experience were reported by both practitioners and experts, with an average of 20 (SD = 10.2) practitioners and 4.6 (SD= 4.3) experts reporting each category. As reported above, similarities between practitioner and experts reports were not impacted by the 11 participants who provided both practitioner and expert interviews.

References:

Agbabiaka, T. B., Savovic, J., & Ernst, E. (2008). Methods for causality assessment of adverse drug reactions: a systematic review. Drug Saf, 31(1), 21-37.

APA. (1994). 300.6 Depersonalization Disorder. In Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM IV) (4th Edition ed.). Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association.

APA. (2013). 300.6 Depersoanlization/Derealization Disorder. In Diagnostic and Staistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Edition (DSM-5) (pp. 302-306). Arlington VA: American Psychiatric Association.

Ataria, Y., Dor-Ziderman, Y., & Berkovich-Ohana, A. (2015). How does it feel to lack a sense of boundaries? A case study of a long-term mindfulness meditator. Consciousness and Cognition, 37, 133-147. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2015.09.002

Berkovich-Ohana, A., Dor-Ziderman, Y., Glicksohn, J., & Goldstein, A. (2013). Alterations in the sense of time, space, and body in the mindfulness-trained brain: a neurophenomenologically-guided MEG study. Front Psychol, 4, 912. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00912

Boorstein, S. (1996). Clinical aspects of meditation. In B. Scotton, A. Chinen, & J. Battista (Eds.), Textbook of Transpersonal Psychiatry and Psychology. New York: Basic Books.

Brown, D., Forte, M., & Dysart, M. (1984). Visual sensitivity and mindfulness meditation. Percept Mot Skills, 58(3), 775-784. doi:10.2466/pms.1984.58.3.775

Carrington, P. (1977). The Misuse of Meditation: Problems from Overmeditation. In Freedom in Meditation. Garden City, NY: Anchor Books.

Castillo, R. (1990). Depersonalization and meditation. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging Section, 53, 158-168.

Chan-Ob, T., & Boonyanaruthee, V. (1999). Meditation in association with psychosis. Journal of the Medical Association of Thailand, 82(9), 925-930.

Craven, J. L. (1989). Meditation and psychotherapy. Can J Psychiatry, 34(7), 648-653.

Deikman, A. (1966). Implications of experimentally induced contemplative meditation. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 142, 101-116.

Disayavanish, C., & Disayavanish, P. (1984). Meditation-induced psychosis (in Thai). . Journal of the Psychiatric Association of Thailand, 29, 1-12.

Dobkin, P., Irving, J., & Amar, S. (2012). For Whom May Participation in a Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction Program be Contraindicated? Mindfulness, 3, 44-50.

Dor-Ziderman, Y., Berkovich-Ohana, A., Glicksohn, J., & Goldstein, A. (2013). Mindfulness-induced selflessness: a MEG neurophenomenological study. Front Hum Neurosci, 7, 582. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00582

Droit-Volet, S., Fanget, M., & Dambrun, M. (2015). Mindfulness meditation and relaxation training increases time sensitivity. Conscious Cogn, 31, 86-97. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2014.10.007

Epstein, M., & Lieff, J. (1981). Psychiatric complications of meditation practice. The Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 13(2), 137-147.

Fenwick, P. (1983). Can we still recommend meditation? Br Med J (Clin Res Ed), 287(6403), 1401.

Gallagher, R. M., Kirkham, J. J., Mason, J. R., Bird, K. A., Williamson, P. R., Nunn, A. J., . . . Pirmohamed, M. (2011). Development and inter-rater reliability of the Liverpool adverse drug reaction causality assessment tool. PLoS One, 6(12), e28096. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0028096

Hanley, A., Abell, N., Osborn, D., Roehrig, A., & Canto, A. (2016). Mind the Gaps: Are Conclusions About Mindfulness Entirely Conclusive? Journal of Counseling & Development, 94, 103-113.

Heide, F., & Borkovec, T. (1983). Relaxation-induced anxiety: paradoxical anxiety enhancement due to relaxation treatment. J Consult Clin Psychol, 51(2), 171-182.

Hill, A. B. (1965). The Environment and Disease: Association or Causation? Proc R Soc Med, 58, 295-300.

Hill, A. B. (2015). The environment and disease: association or causation? J R Soc Med, 108(1), 32-37. doi:10.1177/0141076814562718

Kerr, C. E., Josyula, K., & Littenberg, R. (2011). Developing an observing attitude: an analysis of meditation diaries in an MBSR clinical trial. Clin Psychol Psychother, 18(1), 80-93. doi:10.1002/cpp.700

Kornfield, J. (1979). Intensive insight meditation: A phenomenological study. Journal of Transpersonal Psychology.

Kuijpers, H., van der Heijden, F., Tuinier, S., & Verhoeven, W. (2007). Meditation-induced psychosis. Psychopathology, 40, 461-464.

Kutz, I., Leserman, J., Dorrington, C., Morrison, C. H., Borysenko, J. Z., & Benson, H. (1985). Meditation as an adjunct to psychotherapy. An outcome study. Psychother Psychosom, 43(4), 209-218.

Kuyken, W., Crane, W., & Williams, J. M. (2012). Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy (MBCT) Implementation Resources. Oxford University, University of Exeter, Bangor University.

Lomas, T., Cartwright, T., Edginton, T., & Ridge, D. (2014). A qualitative summary of experiential challenges associated with meditation practice. Mindfulness, 1-13. doi:10.1007/s12671-014-0329-8

Lukoff, D., Lu, F. G., & Turner, R. (1995). Cultural considerations in the assessment and treatment of religious and spiritual problems. Psychiatr Clin North Am, 18(3), 467-485.

Lustyk, M., Chawla, N., Nolan, R., & Marlatt, G. (2009). Mindfulness Medtation Research: Issues of participant screening, safety procedures, and researcher training. Advances in Mind-Body Medicine, 24(1), 20-30.

Miller, J. (1993). The unveling of traumatic memories and emotions through mindfulness and concentration meditation: clinical implications and three case reports. Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 25, 169-180.

Nakaya, M., & Ohmori, K. (2010). Psychosis induced by spiritual practice and resolution of pre-morbid inner conflicts. German Journal of Psychiatry, 13, 161-163.

Naranjo, C. A. (1986). A clinical pharmacologic perspective on the detection and assessment of adverse drug reactions. Drug Inf J, 20(4), 387-393.

Naranjo, C. A., Busto, U., & Sellers, E. M. (1982). Difficulties in assessing adverse drug reactions in clinical trials. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry, 6(4-6), 651-657.

Naranjo, C. A., Busto, U., Sellers, E. M., Sandor, P., Ruiz, I., Roberts, E. A., . . . Greenblatt, D. J. (1981). A method for estimating the probability of adverse drug reactions. Clin Pharmacol Ther, 30(2), 239-245.

NIH. (2016). Adverse Event and Serious Adverse Event Guidelines. In OHRP Guidance on Reviewing and Reporting Unanticipated Problems Involving Risks to Subjects or Others and Adverse Events, OHRP Guidance. National Insitutes of Health (NIH): Office for Human Research Protections, U.S. Department of health and Human Servcies.

OHRP. (2007). Guidance on Reviewing and Reporting Unanticipated Problems Involving Risks to Subjects or Others and Adverse Events: Office for Human Research Protections, US Department of Health and Human Services.

Perez-de-Albeniz, A., & Holmes, J. (2000). Meditation: Concepts, effects and uses in therapy. International Journal of Psychotherapy, 5, 49-58.

Sethi, S. (2003). Relationship of meditation and psychosis: case studies. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 37(3), 382.

Shapiro, D. H., Jr. (1982). Overview: clinical and physiological comparison of meditation with other self-control strategies. Am J Psychiatry, 139(3), 267-274.

Shapiro, D. H., Jr. (1992). Adverse effects of meditation: a preliminary investigation of long-term meditators. Int J Psychosom, 39(1-4), 62-67.

Shonin, E., Van Gordon, W., & Griffiths, M. (2014a). Are there risks associated with using mindfulness in the treatment of psychopathology? Clinical Practice ,, 11, 389-392.

Shonin, E., Van Gordon, W., & Griffiths, M. D. (2014b). Do mindfulness-based therapies have a role in the treatment of psychosis? Aust N Z J Psychiatry, 48(2), 124-127. doi:10.1177/0004867413512688

Shonin E, V. G. W., Griffiths MD. (2014). Cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) and meditation awareness training (MAT) for the treatment of co-occurring schizophrenia with pathological gambling: a case study. Int. J. Ment. Health Addict., 12, 181–196

Theophile, H., Arimone, Y., Miremont-Salame, G., Moore, N., Fourrier-Reglat, A., Haramburu, F., & Begaud, B. (2010). Comparison of three methods (consensual expert judgement, algorithmic and probabilistic approaches) of causality assessment of adverse drug reactions: an assessment using reports made to a French pharmacovigilance centre. Drug Saf, 33(11), 1045-1054. doi:10.2165/11537780-000000000-00000

Turner, R. P., Lukoff, D., Barnhouse, R. T., & Lu, F. G. (1995). Religious or spiritual problem. A culturally sensitive diagnostic category in the DSM-IV. J Nerv Ment Dis, 183(7), 435-444.

Turner, W. M. (1984). The Food and Drug Administration algorithm. Special workshop--regulatory. Drug Inf J, 18(3-4), 259-266.

VAN Nuys, D. (1973). Meditation, attention, and hypnotic susceptibility: A correlation study. . International Journal of Clinical & Experimental Hypnosis, 21, 59-69.

VanderKooi, L. (1997). Buddhist teachers’ experience with extreme mental states in Western meditators. Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 29, 31-46.

Waelde, L. (2004). Dissociation and meditation. Journal of Trauma & Dissociation, 5(2), 147-162.

Walsh, R. (1977). Initial meditative experiences: Part I. Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 9, 151-192.

Walsh, R. (1978). Initial meditative experiences: Part II. Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 10, 1-28.

Walsh, R., & Roche, L. (1979). Precipitation of acute psychotic episodes by intensive meditation in individuals with a history of schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry, 136(8), 1085-1086.

WHO. (2016). The use of the WHO-UMC system for standardized case causaility assessment. who-umc.org: World Health Organization (WHO), Uppsala Monitoring Centre

Yorston, G. (2001). Mania precipitated by meditation: a case report and literature review. Mental Health, Religion & Culture, 4, 209-214.